References

Abner, R.T., Edwards, T., Douglas, A., and Brunner, D., Pharmacology of temporal cognition in two mouse strains, Int. J. Comp. Psychol., 14, 189-210, 2001.

Allada, R., Emery, P., Takahashi, J., and Rosbash, M., Stopping time: the genetics of fly and mouse circadian clocks, Annu. Rev. Neurosci, 24, 1091-1119, 2001.

Allan, L.G., The influence of the scalar timing model on human timing research, Behav. Process, 44, 101-117, 1998.

Allers, K.A., Ruskin, D.N., Bergstrom, D.A., Freeman, L.E., Ghazi, L.J., Tierney, P.L., and Walters, J.R., Multisecond periodicities in basal ganglia firing rates correlate with theta bursts in transcortical and hippocampal EEG, J. Neurophysiol, 87, 1118-1122, 2002.

Arcediano, F. and Miller, R.R., Some constraints for models of timing: a temporal coding hypothesis perspective, Learn. Motiv., 33, 105-123, 2002.

Balsam, P., Relative time in trace conditioning, in Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: Timing and Time Perception, Vol. 423, Gibbon, J. and Allan, L., Eds., New York Academy of Sciences, New York, 1984, pp. 211-227.

Balsam, P.D., Drew, M.R., and Yang, C., Timing at the start of associative learning, Learn. Motiv., 33, 141-155, 2002.

Bao, S., Chen, L., Kim, J.J., and Thompson, R.F., Cerebellar cortical inhibition and classical eyeblink conditioning, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 99, 1592-1597, 2002.

Bateson, M. and Kacelnik, A., Starling's preferences for predictable and unpredictable delays to food, Anim. Behav., 53, 1129-1142, 1997.

Bateson, M. and Kacelnik, A., Risk-sensitive foraging: decision making in variable environments, in Cognitive Ecology: The Evolutionary Ecology of Information Processing and Decision Making, Dukas, R., Ed., Chicago University Press, Chicago, 1998.

Blakeslee, S., Running late? Researchers blame aging brain, The New York Times: Science Times, March 24, 1998, pp. B13, B16.

Botez, M.I., Leveille, J., Lambert, R., and Botez, T., Single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) in cerebellar disease: cerebello-cerebral diaschisis, Eur. Neurol, 31, 405-412, 1991.

Brannon, E.M. and Terrace, H.S., Ordering of the numerosities 1 to 9 by monkeys, Science, 282, 746-749, 1998.

Brannon, E.M., Wusthoff, C.J., Gallistel, C.R., and Gibbon, J., The subjective scaling of number representation, Psychol. Sci., 12, 238-243, 2001.

Breukelaar, J.W.C. and Dalrymple-Alford, J.C., Effects of lesions to the cerebellar vermis and hemispheres on timing and counting in rats, Behav. Neurosci, 113, 78-90, 1999.

Broadbent, H.A., Rakitin, B.C., Church, R.M., and Meck, W.H., Quantitative relationships between timing and counting, in Numerical Skills in Animals, Boysen, S. and Capaldi, E.J., Eds., Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1993, pp. 171-187.

Brockie, P.J., Mellem, J.E., Hills, T., Madsen, D.M., and Maricq, A.V., The C. elegans glutamate receptor subunit NMR-1 is required for slow NMDA-activated currents that regulate reversal frequency during locomotion, Neuron, 31, 617-630, 2001.

Brown, G.D.A. and Chater, N., The chronological organization of memory: common psychological foundations for remembering and timing, in Time and Memory: Issues in Philosophy and Psychology, Hoerl, C. and McCormack, T., Eds., Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2001, pp. 77-110.

Brown, G.D.A., Preece, T., and Hulme, C., Oscillator-based memory for serial order, Psychol. Rev, 107, 127-181, 2000.

Bugmann, G., Towards a neural model of timing, Biosystems, 48, 11-19, 1998.

Buhusi, C.V. and Meck, W.H., Timing for the absence of a stimulus: the gap paradigm reversed, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process., 26, 305-322, 2000.

Buhusi, C.V. and Meck, W.H., Differential effects of methamphetamine and haloperidol on the control of an internal clock, Behav. Neurosci, 116, 291-297, 2002.

Burle, B. and Bonnet, M., What's an internal clock for? From temporal information processing to temporal processing of information, Behav. Process, 45, 59-72, 1999.

Carboni, E., Spielewoy, C., Vacca, C., Nosten-Bertrand, M., Giros, B., and Di Chiara, G., Cocaine and amphetamine increase extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens of mice lacking the dopamine transporter gene, J. Neurosci, 21, RC14, 2001.

Carey, S., On the very possibility of discontinuities in conceptual development, in Language, Brain, and Cognitive Development: Essays in Honor of Jacques Mehler, Dupoux, E., Ed., MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 2001, pp. 303-324.

Casini, L. and Ivry, R.B., Effects of divided attention on temporal processing in patients with lesions of the cerebellum or frontal lobe, Neuropsychology, 13, 10-21, 1999.

Casini, L. and Macar, F., Can the level of prefrontal activity provide an index of performance in humans? Neurosci. Lett, 219, 71-74, 1996a.

Casini, L. and Macar, F., Prefrontal slow way potential in temporal compared to nontemporal tasks, J. Psychophysiol., 10, 252-264, 1996b.

Casini, L. and Macar, F., Multiple approaches to investigate the existence of an internal clock using attentional resources, Behav. Process., 45, 73-85, 1999.

Catania, A.C., Reinforcement schedules and psychophysical judgements: a study of some temporal properties of behavior, in The Theory of Reinforcement Schedules, Schoe-nfeld, W.N., Ed., Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, 1970, pp. 1-42.

Cheng, K. and Westwood, R., Analysis of single trials in pigeons' timing performance, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process, 19, 56-67, 1993.

Cheng, K., Westwood, R., and Crystal, J.D., Memory variance in the peak procedure of timing in pigeons, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process., 19, 68-76, 1993.

Church, R.M., Theories of timing behavior, in Contemporary Learning Theories: Instrumental Conditioning Theory and the Impact of Biological Constraints on Learning, Klein, S.B. and Mowrer, R.R., Eds., Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1989, pp. 41-71.

Church, R.M. and Meck, W.H., The numerical attribute of stimuli, in Animal Cognition, Roitblat, H.L., Bever, T.G., and Terrace, H.S., Eds., Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1984, pp. 445-464.

Church, R.M. and Meck, W.H., Biological basis of the remembered time of reinforcement, in Quantitative Analyses of Behavior: Biological Determinants of Reinforcement, Vol. 7, Commons, M.L., Church, R.M., Stellar, J.R., and Wagner, A.R., Eds., Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1988, pp. 103-119.

Church, R.M., Meck, W.H., and Gibbon, J., Application of scalar timing theory to individual trials, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process, 20, 135-155, 1994.

Clarke, S., Ivry, R., Grinband, J., Roberts, S., and Shimizu, N., Exploring the domain of cerebellar timing, in Time, Internal Clocks and Movement, Pastor, M.A. and Artieda, J., Eds., Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1996, pp. 257-280.

Clement, A. and Droit-Volet, S., Simultaneous or Dissociative Processing of Duration and Number of Sequence of Events? A Developmental Perspective, paper presented at the ESCOP/British Psychological Society Conference, London, UK, September 2002.

Cohen, R.A., Barnes, J., Jenkins, M., and Albers, H.E., Disruption of short-duration timing associated with damage to the suprachiasmatic region of the hypothalamus, Neurology, 48, 1533-1539, 1997.

Constantinidis, C., Williams, G.V., and Goldman-Rakic, P.S., A role for inhibition in shaping the temporal flow of information in prefrontal cortex, Nat. Neurosci, 5, 75-80, 2002.

Cronin-Golomb, A., Corkin, S., and Growdon, J.H., Impaired problem solving in Parkinson's disease: impact of a set-shifting deficit, Neuropsychologia, 32, 579-593, 1994.

Crystal, J.D., Systematic nonlinearities in the perception of temporal intervals, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process, 25, 3-17, 1999.

Crystal, J.D., Circadian time perception, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process., 27, 68-78, 2001.

Crystal, J.D., Church, R.M., and Broadbent, H.A., Systematic nonlinearities in the memory representation of time, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process., 23, 267-282, 1997.

Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Pinel, P., Stanescu, R., and Tsivkin, S., Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence, Science, 284, 970-974, 1999.

Droit-Volet, S., The estimation in young children: an initial force rule governing time production, J. Exp. Child Psychol., 68, 236-249, 1998.

Droit-Volet, S. and Wearden J.H., Temporal bisection in children, J. Exp. Child Psychol, 80, 142-159, 2001.

Egan, M.F., Goldberg, T.E., Kolachana, B.S., Callicott, J.H., Mazzanti, C.M., Straub, R.E., Goldman, D., and Weinberger, D.R., Effect of COMT Val108/158 met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia, Proceed. Nat. Acad. Sci., 98, 6917-6922, 2001.

Fee, M.S., Hahnloser, R.H.R., and Kozhevnikov, A.A., The origin of a representation of time in the brain, Soc. Neurosci. Abstr, 28, 680-683, 2002.

Ferguson, A.A., Cada, A.M., Gray, E.P., and Paule, M.G., No alterations in the performance of two interval timing operant tasks after a-difluoromethylornithine (DFMO)-induced cerebellar stunting, Behav. Brain Res., 126, 135-146, 2001.

Ferrandez, A.M. and Pouthas, V., Does cerebral activity change in middle-aged adults in a visual discrimination task? Neurobiol. Aging, 22, 645-657, 2001.

Fortin, C. and Massé, N., Order information in short-term memory and time estimation, Mem. Cognit, 27, 54-62, 1999.

Fortin, C. and Massé, N., Expecting a break in time estimation: attentional time-sharing without concurrent processing, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 26, 1788-1796, 2000.

Fortin, C. and Rousseau, R., Time estimation as an index of processing demand in memory search, Percept. Psychophys, 42, 377-382, 1987.

Fraisse, P., The Psychology of Time, Harper, New York, 1963.

Gabrieli, J.D., Singh, J., Stebbins, G.T., and Goetz, C.G., Reduced working memory span in Parkinson's disease: evidence for the role of a frontostriatal system in working and strategic memory, Neuropsychology, 10, 322-332, 1996.

Gainetdinov, R.R. and Caron, M.G., Genetics of childhood disorders: hyperdopaminergic mice as an animal model of ADHD, J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 40, 380-382, 2001.

Gainetdinov, R.R., Mohn, A.R., Bohn, L.M., and Caron, M.G., Glutamatergic modulation of hyperactivity in mice lacking the dopamine transporter, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 98, 11047-11054, 2001a.

Gainetdinov, R.R., Mohn, A.R., and Caron, M.G., Genetic animal models: focus on schizophrenia, Trends Neurosci., 24, 527-533, 2001b.

Gainetdinov, R.R., Wetsel, W.C., Jones, S.R., Levin, E.D., Jaber, M., and Caron, M., Role of serotonin in the paradoxical calming effect of psychostimulants on hyperactivity, Science, 282, 397-401, 1999.

Gallistel, C.R., The Organization of Learning, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1990.

Gallistel, C.R. and Gelman, R., Preverbal and verbal counting and computation, Cognition, 44, 43-74, 1992.

Gallistel, C.R. and Gelman, R., Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers, Trends Cognit. Sci., 4, 59-65, 2000.

Gallistel, C.R. and Gibbon, J., Time, rate, and conditioning, Psychol. Rev., 107, 289-344, 2000.

Gallistel, C.R. and Gibbon, J., Computational versus associative models of simple conditioning, Curr. Directions Psychol., 10, 146-150, 2001.

Gautier, T. and Droit-Volet, S., Attentional distraction and time perception in children, Int. J. Psychol, 37, 27-34, 2002.

Gelman, R. and Cordes, S., Counting in animals and humans, in Language, Brain, and Cognitive Development: Essays in Honor of Jacques Mehler, Dupoux, E., Ed., MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 2001, pp. 279-301.

Gibbon, J., Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing, Psychol. Rev, 84, 279-325, 1977.

Gibbon, J., Origins of scalar timing, Learn. Motiv., 22, 3-38, 1991.

Gibbon, J., Ubiquity of scalar timing with a Poisson clock, J. Math. Psychol, 36, 283-293, 1992.

Gibbon, J. and Allan, L., Timing and Time Perception, New York Academy of Sciences, New York, 1984.

Gibbon, J., Baldock, M.D., Locurto, C., Gold, L., and Terrace, H.S., Trial and intertrial durations in autoshaping, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process, 3, 264-284, 1977.

Gibbon, J. and Balsam, P.D., The spread of association in time, in Autoshaping and Conditioning Theory, Locurto, C., Terrace, H.S., and Gibbon, J., Eds., Academic Press, New York, 1981.

Gibbon, J. and Church, R.M., Representation of time, Cognition, 37, 23-54, 1990.

Gibbon, J., Church, R.M., and Meck, W.H., Scalar timing in memory, in Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: Timing and Time Perception, Vol. 423, Gibbon, J. and Allan, L., Eds., New York Academy of Sciences, New York, 1984, pp. 52-77.

Gibbon, J. and Malapani, C., Neural basis of timing and time perception, Encyclopedia Cognit. Sci., in press.

Gibbon, J., Malapani, C., Dale, C.L., and Gallistel, C.R., Toward a neurobiology of temporal cognition: advances and challenges, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 7, 170-184, 1997.

Givens, B.S. and Olton, D.S., Cholinergic and GABAergic modulation of medial septal area: effect on working memory, Behav. Neurosci., 104, 849-855, 1990.

Grondin, S., From physical time to the first and second moments of psychological time, Psychol. Bull., 127, 22-44, 2001.

Grondin, S., Meilleur-Wells, G., and Lachance, R., When to start explicit counting in a time-intervals discrimination task: a critical point in the timing process of humans, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 25, 993-1004, 1999.

Hahnloser, R.H.R., Kozhevnikov, A.A., and Fee, M.S., An ultra-sparse code underlies the generation of neural sequences in a song bird, Nature, 419, 65-70, 2002.

Harrington, D.L. and Haaland, K.Y., Neural underpinnings of temporal processing: a review of focal lesion, pharmacological, and functional imaging research, Rev. Neurosci., 10, 91-116, 1999.

Harrington, D.L., Haaland, K.Y., and Knight, R.T., Cortical networks underlying mechanisms of time perception, J. Neurosci., 18, 1085-1095, 1998.

Hinton, S.H. and Meck, W.H., How time flies: functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing, in Time and Behaviour: Psychological and Neurobiological Analyses, Brad-shaw, C.M. and Szabadi, E., Eds., Elsevier, New York, 1997a, pp. 409-457.

Hinton, S.H. and Meck, W.H., The "internal clocks" of circadian and interval timing, Endeavour, 21, 82-87, 1997b.

Hinton, S.C., Meck, W.H., and MacFall, J.R., Peak-interval timing in humans activates frontalstriatal loops, Neuroimage, 3, S224, 1996.

Hopson, J.W., Gap timing and the spectral timing model, Behav. Process, 45, 23-31, 1999.

Houk, J.C., Information processing in modular circuits linking basal ganglia and cerebral cortex, in Models of Information Processing in the Basal Ganglia, Houk, J.C., Davis, J.L., and Beiser, D.G., Eds., MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1995, pp. 3-10.

Ivry, R.B., The representation of temporal information in perception and motor control, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 6, 851-857, 1996.

Ivry, R.B. and Keele, S.W., Timing functions of the cerebellum, J. Cognit. Neurosci, 1, 136-153, 1989.

Ivry, R.B. and Richardson, T.C., Temporal control and coordination: the multiple timer mode, Brain Cognit, 48, 117-132, 2002.

Jenkins, H.M., Time and contingency in classical conditioning, in Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: Timing and Time Perception, Vol. 423, Gibbon, J. and Allan, L., Eds., New York Academy of Sciences, New York, 1984, pp. 228-241.

Junck, L., Gilman, S., Rothley, J.R., Betley, A.T., Koeppe, R.A., and Hichwa, R.D., A relationship between metabolism in frontal lobes and cerebellum in normal subjects studies with PET, J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab., 8, 774-782, 1988.

Kamin, L.J., Predictability, surprise, attention, and conditioning, in Punishment and Aversive Behavior, Campbell, B.A. and Church, R.M., Eds., Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, 1969, pp. 279-296.

Keele, S.W. and Ivry, R.B., Does the cerebellum provide a common computation for diverse tasks? A timing hypothesis, Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 608, 179-211, 1990.

Killeen, P.R., Scalar counters, Learn. Motiv., 33, 63-87, 2002.

King, A., McDonald, R., and Gallistel, C.R., Screening for mice that remember incorrectly, Int. J. Comp. Psychol, 14, 232-257, 2001.

Krampe, R.T., Engbert, R., and Kliegl, R., Representational models and nonlinear dynamics: irreconcilable approaches to human movement timing and coordination or two sides of the same cone? Introduction to the special issue on movement and coordination, Brain Cognit., 48, 1-6, 2002.

Krebs, J.R. and Kacelnik, A., Time horizons of foraging animals, in Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: Timing and Time Perception, Vol. 423, Gibbon, J. and Allan, L., Eds., New York Academy of Sciences, New York, 1984, pp. 278-291.

Lejeune, H., Switching or gating? The attentional challenge in cognitive models of psychological time, Behav. Process, 44, 127-145, 1998.

Lejeune, H., Maquet, P., Bonnet, M., Casini, L., Ferrara, A., Macar, F., Pouthas, V., Timsit-Berthier, M., and Vidal, F., The basic pattern of activation in motor and sensory temporal tasks: positron emission tomography data, Neurosci. Lett, 235, 21-24, 1997.

Levin, E.D., Conners, C.K., Sparrow, E., Hinton, S.C., Erhardt, D., Meck, W.H., Rose, J.E., and March, J., Nicotine effects on adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, Psychopharmacology, 123, 55-63, 1996.

Levin, E.D., Conners, C.K., Silva, D., Hinton, S.C., Meck, W.H., March, J., and Rose, J.E., Transdermal nicotene effects on attention, Psychopharmacology, 140, 135-141, 1998.

Lewis, P.A., Finding the timer, Trends Cognit. Sci., 6, 195-196, 2002.

Liu, J., Head, E., Gharib, A.M., Yuan, W., Ingersoll, R.T., Hagen, T.M., Cotman, C.W., and Ames, B.N., Memory loss in old rats is associated with brain mitochondrial decay and RNA/DNA oxidation: partial reversal by feeding acetyl-L-carnitine and/or R-a-lipoic acid, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 99, 2356-2361, 2002.

Lustig, C. and Meck, W.H., Paying attention to time as one gets older, Psychol. Sci., 12, 478-484, 2001.

Macar, F., Pouthas, V., and Friedman, W.J., Time, Action, and Cognition: Towards Bridging the Gap, Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht, Netherlands, 1992.

Macar, F., Vidal, F., and Bonnet, M., Laplacian derivation of CNV in time programming, in Psychophysiological Brain Research, Vol. 1, Brunia, C.H.M., Gaillard, A.W.K., and Kok, A., Eds., Tilburg University Press, Tilburg, Netherlands, 1990, pp. 69-76.

Malapani, C., Deweer, B., and Gibbon, J., Separating storage from retrieval dysfunction of temporal memory in Parkinson's disease, J. Cognit. Neurosci., 14, 311-322, 2002.

Malapani, C., Dubois, B., Rancurel, G., and Gibbon, J., Cerebellar dysfunctions of temporal processing in the seconds range in humans, Neuroreport, 9, 3907-3912, 1998a.

Malapani, C., Rakitin, B., Meck, W.H., Deweer, B., Dubois, B., and Gibbon, J., Coupled temporal memories in Parkinson's disease: a dopamine-related dysfunction, J. Cognit. Neurosci, 10, 316-331, 1998b.

Maquet, P., Lejeune, H., Pouthas, V., Bonnet, M., Casini, L., Macar, F., Timsit-Berthier, M., Vidal, F., Ferrara, A., Degueldre, C., Quaglia, L., Delfiore, G., Luxen, A., Woods, R., Mazziotta, J.C., and Comar, D., Brain activation induced by estimation of duration: a PET study, Neuroimage, 3, 119-126, 1996.

Maricq, A.V., Roberts, S., and Church, R.M., Methamphetamine and time estimation, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process., 7, 18-30, 1981.

Matell, M.S. and Meck, W.H., Reinforcement-induced within-trial resetting of an internal clock, Behav. Process., 45, 159-171, 1999.

Matell, M.S. and Meck, W.H., Neuropsychological mechanisms of interval timing behaviour, Bioessays, 22, 94-103, 2000.

Mathew, R.J., Wilson, W.H., Turkington, T.G., and Coleman, R.E., Cerebellar activity and disturbed time sense after THC, Brain Res., 797, 183-189, 1998.

McAuley, J.D. and Jones, M.R., Rhythmic expectations in time judgment behavior: implications for entrainment and interval-based models of time perception, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., submitted.

McCormack, T., Brown, G.D.A., Maylor, E.A., Darby, A., and Green, D., Developmental changes in time estimation: comparing childhood and old age, Dev. Psychol., 35, 1143-1155, 1999.

McCormack, T. and Hoerl, C., Eds., Time and Memory, Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2001.

Meck, W.H., Selective adjustment of the speed of internal clock and memory storage processes, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process, 9, 171-201, 1983.

Meck, W.H., Attentional bias between modalities: effect on the internal clock, memory, and decision stages used in animal time discrimination, in Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: Timing and Time Perception, Vol. 423, Gibbon, J. and Allan, L., Eds., New York Academy of Sciences, New York, 1984, pp. 528-541.

Meck, W.H., Postreinforcement signal processing, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process., 11, 52-70, 1985.

Meck, W.H., Affinity for the dopamine D2 receptor predicts neuroleptic potency in decreasing the speed of an internal clock, Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav, 25, 1185-1189, 1986.

Meck, W.H., Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception, Cognit. Brain Res., 3, 227-242, 1996.

Meck, W.H., Application of a mode-control model of temporal integration to counting and timing behaviour, in Time and Behaviour: Psychological and Neurobiological Analyses, Bradshaw, C.M. and Szabadi, E., Eds., Elsevier, New York, 1997, pp. 133-184.

Meck, W.H., Interval timing and genomics: what makes mutant mice tick? Int. J. Comp. Psychol, 14, 211-231, 2001.

Meck, W.H., Choline uptake in the frontal cortex is proportional to the absolute error of a temporal memory translation constant in mature and aged rats, Learn. Motiv., 33, 88-104, 2002a.

Meck, W.H., Distortions in the content of temporal memory: neurobiological correlates, in Animal Cognition and Sequential Behavior: Behavioral, Biological, and Computational Perspectives, Fountain, S.B., Bunsey, M.D., Danks, J.H., and McBeath, M.K., Eds., Kluwer Academic Press, Boston, 2002b, pp. 175-200.

Meck, W.H. and Benson, A.M., Dissecting the brain's internal clock: how frontal-striatal circuitry keeps time and shifts attention, Brain Cognit., 48, 195-211, 2002.

Meck, W.H. and Church, R.M., A mode control model of counting and timing processes, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process., 9, 320-334, 1983.

Meck, W.H. and Church, R.M., Simultaneous temporal processing, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process., 10, 1-29, 1984.

Meck, W.H. and Church, R.M., Cholinergic modulation of the content of temporal memory, Behav. Neurosci., 101, 457-464, 1987.

Meck, W.H., Church, R.M., and Gibbon, J., Temporal integration in duration and number discrimination, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process., 11, 591-597, 1985.

Meck, W.H., Church, R.M., and Wenk, G.L., Arginine vasopressin inoculates against age-related increases in sodium-dependent high affinity choline uptake and discrepancies in the content of temporal memory, Eur. J. Pharmacol, 130, 327-331, 1986.

Meck, W.H., Church, R.M., Wenk, G.L., and Olton, D.S., Nucleus basalis magnocellularis and medial septal area lesions differentially impair temporal memory, J. Neurosci., 7, 3505-3511, 1987.

Meck, W.H., Hinton, S.C., and Matell, M.S., Coincidence-detection models of interval timing: evidence from fMRI studies of cortico-striatal circuits, Neuroimage, 7, S281, 1998.

Miall, C., The storage of time intervals using oscillating neurons, Neural Comput., 1, 359-371, 1989.

Miall, C., Models of neural timing, in Time, Internal Clocks and Movement, Pastor, A. and Artieda, J., Eds., North-Holland/Elsevier Science, Amsterdam, 1996, pp. 69-94.

Miall, R.C. and Reckless, G.Z., The cerebellum and the timing of coordinated eye and hand tracking, Brain Cognit, 48, 212-226, 2002.

Migliore, M., Messineo, L., Cardaci, M., and Ayala, G.F., Quantitative modeling of perception and production of time intervals, J. Neurophysiol., 86, 2754-2760, 2001.

Monfort, V., Pouthas, V., and Ragot, R., Role of frontal cortex in memory for duration: an event-related potential study in humans, Neurosci. Lett, 286, 91-94, 2000.

Morell, V., Setting a biological stopwatch, Science, 271, 905-906, 1996.

Nichelli, P., Venneri, A., Molinari, M., Tavani, F., and Grafman, J., Precision and accuracy of subjective time estimation in different memory disorders, Cognit. Brain Res., 1, 87-93, 1993.

Ohyama, T., Horvitz, J.C., Gibbon, J., Malapani, C., and Balsam, P.D., Conditioned and unconditioned behavioral-cognitive effects of a dopamine antagonist in rats, Behav. Neurosci, 114, 1251-1255, 2000.

Olton, D.S., Meck, W.H., and Church, R.M., Separation of hippocampal and amygdaloid involvement in temporal memory dysfunctions, Brain Res., 404, 180-188, 1987.

Olton, D.S., Wenk, G.L., Church, R.M., and Meck, W.H., Attention and the frontal cortex as examined by simultaneous temporal processing, Neuropsychologia, 26, 307-318, 1988.

Pang, K.C., Yoder, R.M., and Olton, D.S., Neurons in the lateral agranular frontal cortex have divided attention correlates in a simultaneous temporal processing task, Neuroscience, 103, 615-628, 2001.

Pashler, H., Perception and production of brief durations: beat-based versus interval-based timing, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 27, 485-493, 2001.

Paule, M.G., Meck, W.H., McMillan, D.E., Bateson, M., Popke, E.J., Chelonis, J.J., and Hinton, S.C., The use of timing behaviors in animals and humans to detect drug and/or toxicant effects, Neurotoxicol. Teratol., 21, 491-502, 1999.

Penney, T.B., Gibbon, J., and Meck, W.H., Differential effects of auditory and visual signals on clock speed and temporal memory, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 26, 1770-1787, 2000.

Penney, T.B., Holder, M.D., and Meck, W.H., Clonidine-induced antagonism of norepinephrine modulates the attentional processes involved in peak-interval timing, Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol, 4, 82-92, 1996.

Perbal, S., Ehrle, N., Samson, S., Baulac, M., and Pouthas, V., Time estimation in patients with right or left medial-temporal lobe resection, Neuroreport, 12, 939-942, 2001.

Perrett, S.P., Temporal discrimination in the cerebellar cortex during conditioned eyelid responses, Exp. Brain Res, 121, 115-124, 1998.

Pouthas, V., Garnero, L., Ferrandez, A.M., and Renault, B., ERPs and PET analysis of time perception: spatial and temporal brain mapping during visual discrimination tasks, Hum. Brain Mapping, 10, 49-60, 2000.

Pouthas, V., Maquet, P., Garnero, L., Ferrandez, A.M., and Renault, B., Neural bases of time estimation: a PET and ERP study, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. Suppl, 50, 598-603, 1999.

Pressing, J., The referential dynamics of cognition and action, Psychol. Rev., 106, 714-747, 1999.

Rakitin, B.C., Gibbon, J., Penney, T.B., Malapani, C., Hinton, S.C., and Meck, W.H., Scalar expectancy theory and peak-interval timing in humans, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process, 24, 15-33, 1998.

Rao, S.M., Mayer, A.R., and Harrington, D.L., The evolution of brain activation during temporal processing, Nat. Neurosci, 4, 317-323 , 2001.

Rescorla, R.A. and Wagner, A.R., A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement, in Classical Conditioning: II. Current Research and Theory, Black, A.H. and Prokasy, W.F., Eds., Appleton-Cen-tury-Crofts, New York, 1972, pp. 64-99.

Richelle, M. and Lejeune, H., Time in Animal Behavior, Pergamon Press, New York, 1980.

Richelle, M. and Lejeune, H., Timing competence and timing performance: a cross-species approach, in Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: Timing and Time Perception, Vol. 423, Gibbon, J. and Allan, L., Eds., New York Academy of Sciences, New York, 1984, pp. 254-268.

Roberts, S., Isolation of an internal clock, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process, 7, 242-268, 1981.

Roberts, S. and Holder, M.D., The function of time discrimination and classical conditioning, in Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: Timing and Time Perception, Vol. 423, Gibbon, J. and Allan, L., Eds., New York Academy of Sciences, New York, 1984, pp. 228-241.

Roberts, W.A., Simultaneous numerical and temporal processing in the pigeon, Curr. Directions Psychol. Sci., 4, 47-51, 1995.

Roberts, W.A. and Mitchell, S., Can a pigeon simultaneously process temporal and numerical information? J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process, 20, 66-78, 1994.

Rousseau, R., Picard, D., and Pitre, E., An adaptive counter model for time estimation, in Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: Timing and Time Perception, Vol. 423, Gibbon, J. and Allan, L., Eds., New York Academy of Sciences, New York, 1984, pp. 639-642.

Rousseau, L. and Rousseau, R., Stop-reaction time and the internal clock, Percept. Psycho-phys, 58, 434-448, 1996.

Ruskin, D.N, Bergstrom, D.A., Kaneoke, Y., Patel, B.N., Twery, M.J., and Walters, J.R., Multisecond oscillations in firing rate in the basal ganglia: robust modulation by dopamine receptor activation and anesthesia, J. Neurophysiol., 81 2046-2055, 1999.

Sasaki, A., Andrews, J.R., Higashikubo, B.T., and Meck, W.H., Gene-dose dependent effects on interval timing in dopamine-transporter knockout mice, Soc. Neurosci. Abstr., 28, 183.12, 2002.

Schultz, W., Apicella, P., and Ljungberg, T., Responses of monkey dopamine neurons to reward and conditioned stimuli during successive steps of learning a delayed response task, J. Neurosci., 13, 900-913, 1993.

Schultz, W., Dayan, P., and Montague, P.R., A neural substrate of prediction and reward, Science, 275, 1593-1599, 1997.

Skinner, B.F., The effect on the amount of conditioning of an interval of time before reinforcement, J. Gen. Psychol, 14, 279-295, 1936.

Staddon, J.E.R. and Higa, J.J., Multiple time scales in simple habituation, Psychol. Rev, 103, 720-733, 1996.

Staddon, J.E.R. and Higa, J.J., Time and memory: towards a pacemaker-free theory of interval timing, J. Exp. Anal. Behav, 71, 215-251, 1999.

Stahl, S.M., Essential Psychopharmacology: Neuroscientific Basis and Practical Applications, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England, 2000.

Starkey, P., Spelke, E.S., and Gelman, R., Numerical abstraction by human infants, Cognition, 36, 97-127, 1990.

Stebbins, G.T., Gabrieli, J.D., Masciari, F., Monti, L., and Goetz, C.G., Delayed recognition memory in Parkinson's disease: a role for working memory? Neuropsychologia, 37, 503-510, 1999.

Travis, J., Biological stopwatch found in the brain, Sci. News, 149, 101, 1996.

Vanneste, S. and Pouthas, V., Timing in aging: the role of attention, Exp. Aging Res., 25, 49-67, 1999.

Vidal, F., Bonnet, M., and Macar, F., Can duration be a relevant dimension of motor programs? in Time, Action, and Cognition: Towards Bridging the Gap, Macar, F., Pouthas, V., and Friedman, W.J., Eds., Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht, Netherlands, 1992.

Wagner, A.R., SOP: a model of automatic memory processing in animal behavior, in Information Processing in Animals: Memory Mechanisms, Spear, N.E. and Miller, R.R., Eds., Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1981., pp. 5-47.

Wearden, J.H., "Beyond the fields we know ... ": exploring and developing scalar timing theory, Behav. Process, 45, 3-21, 1999.

Whalen, J., Gallistel, C.R., and Gelman, R., Non-verbal counting in humans: the psychophys-ics of number representation, Psychol. Sci., 10, 130-137, 1999.

Woodruff-Pak, D.S. and Papka, M., Huntington's disease and eyeblink conditioning: normal learning but abnormal timing, J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc., 2, 323-334, 1996.

Wright, K., Times of our lives, Sci. Am., 9, 58-65, 2002.

Wynn, K., Addition and subtraction by human infants, Nature, 358, 749-750, 1992.

Wynn, K., Infants possess a system of numerical knowledge, Curr. Directions Psychol. Sci., 4, 172-177, 1995.

Wynn, K., Psychological foundations of number: numerical competence in human infants, Trends Cognit. Sci., 2, 296-303, 1998.

Wynn, K. and Chiang, W-C., Limits to infants' knowledge of objects: the case of magical appearance, Psychol. Sci., 9, 448-455, 1998.

Yu, A.C. and Margoliash, D., Temporal hierarchical control of singing in birds, Science, 273, 1871-1875, 1996.

Zakay, D. and Block, R.A., The role of attention in time estimation processes, in Time, Internal Clocks and Movement, Pastor, M.A. and Artieda, J., Eds., Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1996, pp. 143-164.

Warren H. Meck

Duke University Durham, North Carolina January 9, 2003

The ADHD Success Formula

The ADHD Success Formula

This is an audio and guide that will help you battle through ADHD and Accomplish Twice As Much In Half The Time. Learn more by download your very own copy today.

Get My Free Ebook


Post a comment