References

Allan, L.G., The influence of the scalar timing model on human timing research, Behav.

Process., 44, 101-117, 1998. Aschoff, J., Circadian timing, in Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: Timing and Time Perception, Vol. 423, Gibbon, J. and Allan, L., Eds., New York Academy of Sciences, New York, 1984, pp. 442-468. Aschoff, J., On the perception of time during prolonged temporal isolation, Hum. Neurobiol., 4, 41-52, 1985a.

Aschoff, J., Time perception and timing of meals during temporal isolation, in Circadian Clocks and Zeitgebers, Hiroshige, T. and Honma, K., Eds., Hokkaido University Press, Sapporo, Japan, 1985b, pp. 3-18. Aschoff, J. and Daan, S., Human time perception in temporal isolation: effects of illumination intensity, Chronobiol. Int., 14, 585-596, 1997. Aschoff, J., von Goetz, C., and Honma, K.I., Restricted feeding in rats: effects of varying feeding cycles, Z. Tierpsychol., 63, 91-111, 1983. Bolles, R.C. and de Lorge, J., The rat's adjustment to a-diurnal feeding cycles, J. Comp.

Physiol. Psychol., 55, 760-762, 1962. Bolles, R.C. and Stokes, L.W., Rat's anticipation of diurnal and a-diurnal feeding, J. Comp.

Physiol. Psychol., 60, 290-294, 1965. Boulos, Z., Rosenwasser, A.M., and Terman, M., Feeding schedules and the circadian organization of behavior in the rat, Behav. Brain Res, 1, 39-65, 1980. Broadbent, H.A., Periodic behavior in a random environment, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav.

Process, 20, 156-175, 1994. Brunner, D., Kacelnik, A., and Gibbon, J., Optimal foraging and timing processes in the starling, Sturnus vulgaris: effect of intercapture interval, Anim. Behav., 44, 597-613, 1992.

Church, R.M., The internal clock: introduction, in Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: Timing and Time Perception, Vol. 423, Gibbon, J. and Allan, L., Eds., New York Academy of Sciences, New York, 1984, p. 469. Church, R.M., Quantitative models of animal learning and cognition, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim.

Behav. Process., 23, 379-389, 1997. Church, R.M. and Broadbent, H.A., Alternative representations of time, number, and rate,

Cognition, 37, 55-81, 1990. Church, R.M., Getty, D.J., and Lerner, N.D., Duration discrimination by rats, J. Exp. Psychol.

Anim. Behav. Process., 2, 303-312, 1976. Church, R.M., Meck, W.H., and Gibbon, J., Application of scalar timing theory to individual trials, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process, 20, 135-155, 1994.

Collyer, C.E., Broadbent, H.A., and Church, R.M., Categorical time production: evidence for discrete timing in motor control, Percept. Psychophys., 51, 134-144, 1992.

Collyer, C.E., Broadbent, H.A., and Church, R.M., Preferred rates of repetitive tapping and categorical time production, Percept. Psychophys., 55, 443-453, 1994.

Crystal, J.D., Systematic nonlinearities in the perception of temporal intervals, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process, 25, 3-17, 1999.

Crystal, J.D., Circadian time perception, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process., 27, 68-78, 2001a.

Crystal, J.D., Nonlinear time perception, Behav. Process., 55, 35-49, 2001b.

Crystal, J.D., Timing inter-reward intervals, Learn. Motiv., 33, 311-326, 2002.

Crystal, J.D., Church, R.M., and Broadbent, H.A., Systematic nonlinearities in the memory representation of time, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process., 23, 267-282, 1997.

Dews, P.B., The theory of fixed-interval responding, in The Theory of Reinforcement Schedules, Schoenfeld, W.N., Ed., Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, 1970, pp. 43-61.

Fetterman, J.G. and Killeen, P.R., Time discrimination in Columba livia and Homo sapiens, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process., 18, 80-94, 1992.

Fetterman, J.G. and Killeen, P.R., Categorical scaling of time: implications for clock-counter models, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process, 21, 43-63, 1995.

Gallistel, C.R., The Organization of Learning, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1990.

Gibbon, J., Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing, Psychol. Rev, 84, 279-325, 1977.

Gibbon, J., Origins of scalar timing, Learn. Motiv., 22, 3-38, 1991.

Gibbon, J. and Church, R.M., Sources of variance in an information processing theory of timing, in Animal Cognition, Roitblat, H.L., Bever, T.G., and Terrance, H.S., Eds., Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1984, pp. 456-488.

Gibbon, J., Fairhurst, S., and Goldberg, B., Cooperation, conflict and compromise between circadian and interval clocks in pigeons, in Time and Behavior: Psychological and Neurobehavioural Analyses, Bradshaw, C.M. and Szabadi, E., Eds., Elsevier, New York, 1997a, pp. 329-384.

Gibbon, J., Malapani, C., Dale, C.L., and Gallistel, C.R., Toward a neurobiology of temporal cognition: advances and challenges, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 7, 170-184, 1997b.

Gibbon, J., Morrell, M., and Silver, R., Two kinds of timing in circadian incubation rhythm of ring doves, Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol., 247, 1083-1087, 1984.

Hinton, S.H. and Meck, W.H., The "internal clocks" of circadian and interval timing, Endeavour, 21, 82-87, 1997.

Killeen, P.R., Scalar counters, Learn. Motiv., 33, 63-87, 2002.

Killeen, P.R. and Taylor, T.J., How the propagation of error through stochastic counters affects time discrimination and other psychophysical judgments, Psychol. Rev, 107, 430-459, 2000.

Kirkpatrick-Steger, K., Miller, S.S., Betti, C.A., and Wasserman, E.A., Cyclic responding by pigeons on the peak timing procedure, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process., 22, 447-460, 1996.

Kristofferson, A.B., A quantal step function in duration discrimination, Percept. Psychophys., 27, 300-306, 1980.

Kristofferson, A.B., Quantal and deterministic timing in human duration discrimination, Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci, 423, 3-15, 1984.

Lejeune, H. and Wearden, J.H., The comparative psychology of fixed-interval responding: some quantitative analyses, Learn. Motiv., 22, 84-111, 1991.

Lustig, C. and Meck, W.H., Paying attention to time as one gets older, Psychol. Sci., 12, 478-484, 2001.

Machado, A. and Cevik, M., Acquisition and extinction under periodic reinforcement, Behav. Process., 44, 237-262, 1998.

Macmillan, N.A. and Creelman, C.D., Detection Theory: A User's Guide, Cambridge University Press, New York, 1991.

Madrid, J.A., Sanchez-Vazquez, F.J., Lax, P., Matas, P., Cuenca, E.M., and Zamora, S., Feeding behavior and entrainment limits in the circadian system of the rat, Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol., 275, 372-383, 1998.

Meck, W.H., Modality-specific circadian rhythmicities influence mechanisms of attention and memory for interval timing, Learn. Motiv., 22, 153-179, 1991.

Mistlberger, R.E., Circadian food-anticipatory activity: formal models and physiological mechanisms, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 18, 171-195, 1994.

Mistlberger, R.E. and Marchant, E.G., Computational and entrainment models of circadian food-anticipatory activity: evidence from non-24-hr feeding schedules, Behav. Neurosci., 109, 790-798, 1995.

Pizzo, M.J. and Crystal, J.D., Representation of time in time-place learning, Anim. Learn. Behav., in press.

Rakitin, B.C., Gibbon, J., Penney, T.B., Malapani, C., Hinton, S.C., and Meck, W.H., Scalar expectancy theory and peak-interval timing in humans, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process, 24, 15-33, 1998.

Richelle, M. and Lejeune, H., Timing competence and timing performance: a cross-species approach, Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 423, 254-266, 1984.

Rosenbaum, D.A., Broadcast theory of timing, in Timing of Behavior: Neural, Psychological, and Computational Perspectives, Rosenbaum, D.A. and Collyer, C.E., Eds., MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1998, pp. 215-235.

Stephan, F.K., Limits of entrainment to periodic feeding in rats with suprachiasmatic lesions, J. Comp. Physiol, 143, 401-410, 1981.

Stephan, F.K., Swann, J.M., and Sisk, C.L., Anticipation of 24-hr feeding schedules in rats with lesions of the suprachiasmatic nucleus, Behav. Neural Biol, 25, 346-363, 1979a.

Stephan, F.K., Swann, J.M., and Sisk, C.L., Entrainment of circadian rhythms by feeding schedules in rats with suprachiasmatic lesions, Behav. Neural Biol., 25, 545-554, 1979b.

Terman, M., Gibbon, J., Fairhurst, S., and Waring, A., Daily meal anticipation: interaction of circadian and interval timing, Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 423, 470-487, 1984.

Timberlake, W., Behavior systems and reinforcement: an integrative approach, J. Exp. Anal. Behav, 60, 105-128, 1993.

Timberlake, W., Behavior systems, associationism, and Pavlovian conditioning, Psychonomic Bull. Rev, 1, 405-420, 1994.

Wearden, J.H., Human performance on an analogue of an interval bisection task, Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Comp. Physiol. Psychol, 43, 59-81, 1991.

Wearden, J.H., Rogers, P., and Thomas, R., Temporal bisection in humans with longer stimulus durations, Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Comp. Physiol. Psychol, 50, 79-94, 1997.

White, W. and Timberlake, W., Meal-engendered circadian-ensuing activity in rats, Physiol. Behav., 65, 625-642, 1999.

Zeiler, M.D., Ecological influences on timing, J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process, 17, 13-25, 1991.

Zeiler, M.D. and Powell, D.G., Temporal control in fixed-interval schedules, J. Exp. Anal. Behav, 61, 1-9, 1994.

0 0

Post a comment